Playing an instrument gives better
cognition
Another of the many benefits of a
musical education
THE ECONOMIST 14 Sept 2022
From strumming a guitar next to a campfire to entertaining guests
with a piano piece at a formal dinner, being able to play a musical
instrument is unquestionably rewarding. Yet, evidence suggests that
the rewards go far beyond the elation of performing well in front of
others—those who play instruments have often been found to perform
better on cognitive tests too.
Enhanced cognition is well-known to be linked to a range of
positive life outcomes such as getting a better job and enjoying
improved health. However, it has remained unclear whether these
enhanced cognitive skills are just temporary. New research published
in Psychological Science suggests that the benefits of musical
instruments remain for decades. Studies comparing the mental
abilities of musicians and non-musicians often show that musical
training is related to small, but significant, cognitive benefits
even when confounding factors, such as socioeconomic status, are
accounted for. Findings from experimental studies with children have
also lent support to the idea that musical training might cause an
improvement in cognitive ability. Indeed, there is evidence that
just two years of such training enhances cognition.
Unfortunately, a major limitation of these studies is their
duration. They almost always have short monitoring periods. This is
not because psychologists do not yearn to monitor their participants
for longer. It is more a matter of time and resources. Running
experiments over the course of several decades is logistically
challenging and expensive.
In their latest study, Judith Okely at Edinburgh University
and her colleagues, Ian Deary and Katie Overy, identified a solution
to that age-old problem: the Lothian Birth Cohort. On a single day
in 1947, the Scottish government tested the intelligence of almost
every 11-year-old child who attended school in the country. In 1997
Dr Deary contacted 1,091 of those people and tested them once more
between 2004 and 2007. The study is still ongoing with participants
returning for further cognitive testing every three years.
Although information about musical ability was not initially
collected as part of the study, while pondering the question of how
learning an instrument shaped cognition over time in early 2017, Dr
Overy, a researcher at Edinburgh University’s Reid School of Music,
realised it was not too late to ask the original participants about
their musical experiences. The researchers worked together to
develop a questionnaire that collected information about lifetime
musical experience. This was completed by the surviving cohort
members who returned to the study for further testing at age 82.
The participants were asked how many instruments they played and
what their training was like. They were also asked to record how
many years of regular practice they had and what performance level
(for example beginner, intermediate or advanced) they had reached. A
total of 366 cohort members provided usable information and 117
revealed that they had some degree of experience with musical
instruments. Overall, the researchers found that a significant
positive relationship existed between playing an instrument and
change in cognitive ability over time.
More specifically, the more years and more hours of practice with an
instrument that a person had, the more likely they were to show a
positive cognitive change over the course of their life. The effect
was small but it remained significant even when the findings were
adjusted to take into account confounding factors like years of
education and socioeconomic status.
Precisely why learning to play a musical instrument has these
effects remains unclear. The researchers theorise that driving
people to regularly use a mix of focused attention, co-ordination,
auditory-motor skills and memory results in advantageous cognitive
changes. Yet another reward, then, from a love of music. |